Elsevier

Neurobiology of Aging

Volume 38, February 2016, Pages 205-213
Neurobiology of Aging

Regular article
Neural synchrony indexes impaired motor slowing after errors and novelty following white matter damage

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.10.014Get rights and content

Abstract

In humans, action errors and perceptual novelty elicit activity in a shared frontostriatal brain network, allowing them to adapt their ongoing behavior to such unexpected action outcomes. Healthy and pathologic aging reduces the integrity of white matter pathways that connect individual hubs of such networks and can impair the associated cognitive functions. Here, we investigated whether structural disconnection within this network because of small-vessel disease impairs the neural processes that subserve motor slowing after errors and novelty (post-error slowing, PES; post-novel slowing, PNS). Participants with intact frontostriatal circuitry showed increased right-lateralized beta-band (12–24 Hz) synchrony between frontocentral and frontolateral electrode sites in the electroencephalogram after errors and novelty, indexing increased neural communication. Importantly, this synchrony correlated with PES and PNS across participants. Furthermore, such synchrony was reduced in participants with frontostriatal white matter damage, in line with reduced PES and PNS. The results demonstrate that behavioral change after errors and novelty result from coordinated neural activity across a frontostriatal brain network and that such cognitive control is impaired by reduced white matter integrity.

Introduction

Controlled behavior requires the ability to adapt ongoing actions to unexpected outcomes. For example, when driving a car, both one's own actions (e.g., accidentally switching on the windshield wiper, “error”) and outside factors (e.g., a failing power steering, “novel”) can lead to unexpected action outcomes, which require immediate adaptation of behavior. Over the life span, the capacity to implement such cognitive control processes progressively diminishes (Braver and Barch, 2002, Harty et al., 2013, Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004, Verhaeghen and Cerella, 2002), which is further escalated by pathologic age-related processes, such as small-vessel disease (SVD) (Prins et al., 2005).

In healthy individuals, cognitive control after unexpected action outcomes is well characterized. Both errors and novels lead to slower reaction times on subsequent trials (post-novelty slowing, PNS, Barcelo et al., 2006, Notebaert et al., 2009, Parmentier et al., 2011, post-error slowing, PES, Rabbitt and Rodgers, 1977) and engage a number of prefrontal, cingulate, and basal ganglia brain regions (Hester et al., 2004, Kerns et al., 2004, Polich, 2007, Ridderinkhof et al., 2004, Soltani and Knight, 2000). In fact, errors and novels engage a common distributed brain network (Wessel et al., 2012), which could explain why both types of events evoke similar behavioral changes (PES and PNS). However, it is not clear how exactly PES and PNS are implemented within this network and how healthy or pathologic aging affects these cognitive control abilities.

Recent studies suggest that a right-lateralized frontostriatal motor inhibition mechanism could underlie PES and PNS (Danielmeier et al., 2011, Marco-Pallares et al., 2008, Wessel and Aron, 2013). This motor inhibition mechanism is engaged when actions have to be cancelled (Aron et al., 2007b, Aron et al., 2014) or slowed down (Chikazoe et al., 2009, Jahfari et al., 2010, Wessel and Aron, 2014, Wessel et al., 2013). Importantly, the anatomic regions that subserve this motor inhibition mechanism (right inferior gyrus, presupplemetary motor area, and subthalamic nucleus, Aron et al., 2007a, Rae et al., 2015) are in fact part of the wider frontostriatal network that is active after both errors and novelty (Wessel et al., 2012). Activity of this mechanism can be measured using electroencephalography (EEG). When this mechanism is engaged, neural synchrony between frontocentral and right frontolateral brain areas is increased (specifically within the beta frequency band, i.e., 12–24 Hz, Swann et al., 2011, Swann et al., 2012), which ostensibly reflects communication between distant brain areas (Fries, 2005). Hence, the implementation of PES and PNS could crucially depend on the neural communication between the distant nodes of the brain network underlying this mechanism, which is indexed by right-lateralized beta-band synchrony.

However, testing the causal importance of neural synchrony in humans is difficult. Whereas gray matter damage allows testing the causal role of focal brain areas, testing the causal role of the integrity of a distributed brain network necessitates the investigation of changes in white matter pathways. From this perspective, the aging process allows unique insights. Both healthy (e.g., Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007, Barrick et al., 2010, Davis et al., 2009, Westlye et al., 2010) and pathologic aging (e.g., Damoiseaux et al., 2009, Prins et al., 2005) are characterized by reductions in white matter integrity. Here, we used small-vessel disease (SVD) as a model of structural white matter disconnection to study the causal role of right lateralized beta-band synchrony in PES and PNS. SVD is an aging-related disorder (Pugh and Lipsitz, 2002) characterized by vascular white matter lesions (Cummings, 1995) and lacunar infarcts (Okroglic et al., 2013), which lead to impaired long-range neural communication (Schaefer et al., 2014).

In our present study, EEG was recorded from participants with and without frontostriatal microlesions because of SVD (rated using the age-related white matter changes rating scale, ARWMC, Wahlund et al., 2001) during a hybrid error-novelty task (Wessel et al., 2012, Wessel et al., 2014). We predicted that beta-band synchrony between frontocentral and right frontolateral brain regions would be increased after errors and novelty in participants with intact frontostriatal white matter. We furthermore predicted that such synchrony would be decreased in participants with frontostriatal structural disconnections and that this decrease would be correlated with impaired PES and PNS.

Section snippets

Participants

Twenty-eight native German speakers with corrected or corrected-to-normal vision were recruited from the participant databases of the Day Clinic for Cognitive Neurology in Leipzig and the participant database of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences. We specifically recruited participants for whom we had existing ratings of white matter integrity and neuropsychological testing data (these subjects had been recruited as part of another study, Quinque et al., 2012, for

Neuropsychological measures

The lesion group showed significantly worse performance compared with the control group in verbal fluency (t(22) = 2.62, p = 0.02) and TMT-A (t(22) = 2.18, p = 0.04) and marginally worse performance in phonemic fluency (t(22) = 1.73, p < 0.1) and TMT-B (t(22) = –1.98, p = 0.06). No group differences were found in MMST (t(22) = 0.47, p = 0.64) and the TMT-B/TMT-A quotient (t(22) = 0.22, p = 0.83, Table 2).

Importantly, a comparison of the neuropsychological testing data between the time point of

Discussion

In this study, we show that a right-lateralized mechanism for motor inhibition, whose activity is indexed by EEG coherence between frontocentral and right frontolateral scalp regions, is crucially involved in behavioral change after unexpected action outcomes—namely, PES and PNS of reaction times. The study shows, for the first time, that right-lateralized EEG coherence is increased specifically after errors and novelty. Even more importantly, such right-lateralized EEG beta-band coherence

Conclusions

The present study is important in multiple ways. First, it provides evidence for a right-lateralized neural motor inhibition mechanism underlying the slowing of reaction times after unexpected action outcomes such as errors or perceptual novelty. Second, it shows that the activity of this mechanism can be quantified using event-related right-lateralized EEG beta-band coherence, which varies in amplitude with the degree of PES and PNS. Third, it shows that white matter damage, in our case caused

Disclosure statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Kristiane Klein for helping with electroencephalographic data collection and the patients for participating in this study. JRW has been supported by a grant of the Gertrud Reemtsma Foundation for Brain Research. MLS has been supported by the Parkinson's Disease Foundation (Grant No. PDF-IRG-1307) and by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF, German FTLD Consortium, Grant No. FKZ 01GI1007A). MLS and AV have been supported by LIFE—Leipzig

References (63)

  • P. Fries

    A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2005)
  • B.T. Gold et al.

    Age-related slowing of task switching is associated with decreased integrity of frontoparietal white matter

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2010)
  • J.L. Holtrop et al.

    Investigating age-related changes in fine motor control across different effectors and the impact of white matter integrity

    Neuroimage

    (2014)
  • J. Kayser et al.

    Principal components analysis of Laplacian waveforms as a generic method for identifying ERP generator patterns: I. Evaluation with auditory oddball tasks

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2006)
  • W. Notebaert et al.

    Post-error slowing: an orienting account

    Cognition

    (2009)
  • D. Pinal et al.

    Stuck in default mode: inefficient cross-frequency synchronization may lead to age-related short-term memory decline

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2015)
  • J. Polich

    Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2007)
  • K.G. Pugh et al.

    The microvascular frontal-subcortical syndrome of aging

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2002)
  • N.C. Swann et al.

    Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity

    Neuroimage

    (2012)
  • H.G. Vaughan et al.

    Topography of human motor potential

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1968)
  • P. Verhaeghen et al.

    Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2002)
  • J.R. Wessel et al.

    Inhibitory motor control based on complex stopping goals relies on the same brain network as simple stopping

    Neuroimage

    (2014)
  • J.R. Wessel et al.

    Lesions to the prefrontal performance-monitoring network disrupt neural processing and adaptive behaviors after both errors and novelty

    Cortex

    (2014)
  • D.A. Ziegler et al.

    Cognition in healthy aging is related to regional white matter integrity, but not cortical thickness

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2010)
  • A.R. Aron et al.

    Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI

    J. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • A.R. Aron et al.

    Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia network for inhibitory control of action and cognition

    J. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • F. Barcelo et al.

    Task switching and novelty processing activate a common neural network for cognitive control

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2006)
  • A.J. Bell et al.

    An information maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution

    Neural Comput.

    (1995)
  • Y. Benjamini et al.

    Controlling the false discovery rate—a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing

    J. Roy. Stat. Soc. B Met.

    (1995)
  • J. Chikazoe et al.

    Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a stop-signal task

    J. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • J.P. Coxon et al.

    Aging and inhibitory control of action: cortico-subthalamic connection strength predicts stopping performance

    J. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • Cited by (14)

    • Doing it Wrong: A Systematic Review on Electrocortical and Behavioral Correlates of Error Monitoring in Patients with Neurological Disorders

      2022, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Patients with fronto-striatal white-matter lesions also showed no difference in performance compared with HCs. However, a PES effect was found in HC but not in PD (Wessel et al., 2016). It is also worth noting that Wessel and colleagues (2016) found a significant group difference for the post-novelty slowing (PNS), thus the slowing of reaction times in subsequent trials after novel stimuli that was found in the HCs in comparison to the patients.

    • Mutant Tau knock-in mice display frontotemporal dementia relevant behaviour and histopathology

      2016, Neurobiology of Disease
      Citation Excerpt :

      Clearly, further studies are required, as this approach may provide valuable translational and diagnostic value (Platt and Riedel, 2011; Platt et al., 2011a, 2011b). In particular, novel qEEG parameters and network analyses may offer improved disease-specific criteria (Farb et al., 2013; Wessel et al., 2016). A correlate of changes in hippocampal power spectra may be found within altered synaptic efficiency observed in hippocampal slices from PLB2Tau mice.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    These senior authors contributed equally to this work.

    View full text